2.1 Why plot your data?
Getting information from a table is like extracting sunlight from a cucumber. Farquhar & Farquhar (1891)
At the time the Farhquhar brothers wrote this pithy aphorism, graphical methods for understanding data had advanced considerably, but were not universally practiced, prompting their complaint.
The main graphic forms we use today—the pie chart, line graphs and bar—were invented by William Playfair around 1800 (Playfair, 1786, 1801). The scatterplot arrived shortly after (Herschel, 1833) and thematic maps showing the spatial distributions of social variables (crime, suicides, literacy) were used for the first time to reason about important societal questions (Guerry, 1833) such as “is increased education associated with lower rates of crime?”
In the last half of the 18th Century, the idea of correlation was developed (Galton, 1886; Pearson, 1896) and the period, roughly 1860–1890, dubbed the “Golden Age of Graphics (Funkhouser, 1937) became the richest period of innovation and beauty in the entire history of data visualization. During this time there was an incredible development of visual thinking, represented by the work of Charles Joseph Minard, advances in the role of visualization within scientific discovery, as illustrated through Francis Galton, and graphical excellence, embodied in state statistical atlases produced in France and elsewhere. See Friendly (2008); Friendly & Wainer (2021) for this history.
This chapter introduces the importance of graphing data through three nearly classic stories with the following themes:
summary statistics are not enough: Anscombe’s Quartet demonstrates datasets that are indistinguishable by numerical summary statistics (mean, standard deviation, correlation), but whose relationships are vastly different.
one lousy point can ruin your day: A researcher is mystified by a difference between a correlation for men and women until she plots the data.
finding the signal in noise: The story of the US 1970 Draft Lottery shows how a weak, but reliable signal, reflecting bias in a process can be revealed by graphical enhancement and summarization.
2.1.1 Anscombe’s Quartet
In 1973, Francis Anscombe (Anscombe, 1973) famously constructed a set of four datasets illustrate the importance of plotting the graphs before analyzing and model building, and the effect of unusual observations on fitted models. Now known as Anscombe’s Quartet, these datasets had identical statistical properties: the same means, standard deviations, correlations and regression lines.
His purpose was to debunk three notions that had been prevalent at the time:
- Numerical calculations are exact, but graphs are coarse and limited by perception and resolution;
- For any particular kind of statistical data there is just one set of calculations constituting a correct statistical analysis;
- Performing intricate calculations is virtuous, whereas actually looking at the data is cheating.
The dataset datasets::anscombe
has 11 observations, recorded in wide format, with variables x1:x4
and y1:y4
.
The following code transforms this data to long format and calculates some summary statistics for each dataset
.
anscombe_long <- anscombe |>
pivot_longer(everything(),
names_to = c(".value", "dataset"),
names_pattern = "(.)(.)"
) |>
arrange(dataset)
anscombe_long |>
group_by(dataset) |>
summarise(xbar = mean(x),
ybar = mean(y),
r = cor(x, y),
intercept = coef(lm(y ~ x))[1],
slope = coef(lm(y ~ x))[2]
)
#> # A tibble: 4 × 6
#> dataset xbar ybar r intercept slope
#> <chr> <dbl> <dbl> <dbl> <dbl> <dbl>
#> 1 1 9 7.50 0.816 3.00 0.500
#> 2 2 9 7.50 0.816 3.00 0.5
#> 3 3 9 7.5 0.816 3.00 0.500
#> 4 4 9 7.50 0.817 3.00 0.500
As we can see, all four datasets have nearly identical univariate and bivariate statistical measures. You can only see how they differ in graphs, which show their true natures to be vastly different.
Figure 2.1 is an enhanced version of Anscombe’s plot of these data, adding helpful annotations to show visually the underlying statistical summaries.
This figure is produced as follows, using a single call to ggplot()
, faceted by dataset
. As we will see later (Section 3.2), the data ellipse (produced by stat_ellipse()
) reflects the correlation between the variables.
desc <- tibble(
dataset = 1:4,
label = c("Pure error", "Lack of fit", "Outlier", "Influence")
)
ggplot(anscombe_long, aes(x = x, y = y)) +
geom_point(color = "blue", size = 4) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y ~ x, se = FALSE,
color = "red", linewidth = 1.5) +
scale_x_continuous(breaks = seq(0,20,2)) +
scale_y_continuous(breaks = seq(0,12,2)) +
stat_ellipse(level = 0.5, color=col, type="norm") +
geom_label(data=desc, aes(label = label), x=6, y=12) +
facet_wrap(~dataset, labeller = label_both)
The subplots are labeled with the statistical idea they reflect:
dataset 1: Pure error. This is the typical case with well-behaved data. Variation of the points around the line reflect only measurement error or unreliability in the response, \(y\).
dataset 2: Lack of fit. The data is clearly curvilinear, and would be very well described by a quadratic,
y ~ poly(x, 2)
. This violates the assumption of linear regression that the fitted model has the correct form.dataset 3: Outlier. One point, second from the right, has a very large residual. Because this point is near the extreme of \(x\), it pulls the regression line towards it, as you can see by imagining a line through the remaining points.
dataset 4: Influence. All but one of the points have the same \(x\) value. The one unusual point has sufficient influence to force the regression line to fit it exactly.
One moral from this example:
Linear regression only “sees” a line. It does its’ best when the data are really linear. Because the line is fit by least squares, it pulls the line toward discrepant points to minimize the sum of squared residuals.
The method Anscombe used to compose his quartet is unknown, but it turns out that that there is a method to construct a wider collection of datasets with identical statistical properties. After all, in a bivariate dataset with \(n\) observations, the correlation has \((n-2)\) degrees of freedom, so it is possible to choose \(n-2\) of the \((x, y)\) pairs to yield any given value. As it happens, it is also possible to create any number of datasets with the same means, standard deviations and correlations with nearly any shape you like — even a dinosaur!
The Datasaurus Dozen was first publicized by Alberto Cairo in a blog post and are available in the datasauRus package (Davies et al., 2022). As shown in Figure 2.2, the sets include a star, cross, circle, bullseye, horizontal and vertical lines, and, of course the “dino”. The method (Matejka & Fitzmaurice, 2017) uses simulated annealing, an iterative process that perturbs the points in a scatterplot, moving them towards a given shape while keeping the statistical summaries close to the fixed target value.
The datasauRus package just contains the datasets, but a general method, called statistical metamers, for producing such datasets has been described by Elio Campitelli and implemented in the metamer package.
The essential idea of a statistical “quartet” is to illustrate four quite different datasets or circumstances that seem superficially the same, but yet are paradoxically very different when you look behind the scenes. For example, in the context of causal analysis Gelman et al. (2023), illustrated sets of four graphs, within each of which all four represent the same average (latent) causal effect but with much different patterns of individual effects; McGowan et al. (2023) provide another illustration with four seemingly identical data sets each generated by a different causal mechanism. As an example of machine learning models, Biecek et al. (2023), introduced the “Rashamon Quartet”, a synthetic dataset for which four models from different classes (linear model, regression tree, random forest, neural network) have practically identical predictive performance. In all cases, the paradox is solved when their visualization reveals the distinct ways of understanding structure in the data. The quartets package contains these and other variations on this theme.
2.1.2 One lousy point can ruin your day
In the mid 1980s, a consulting client had a strange problem.1 She was conducting a study of the relation between body image and weight preoccupation in exercising and non-exercising people (Davis, 1990). As part of the design, the researcher wanted to know if self-reported weight could be taken as a reliable indicator of true weight measured on a scale. It was expected that the correlations between reported and measured weight should be close to 1.0, and the slope of the regression lines for men and women should also be close to 1.0. The dataset is car::Davis
.
She was therefore very surprise to see the following numerical results: For men, the correlation was nearly perfect, but not so for women.
Similarly, the regression lines showed the expected slope for men, but that for women was only 0.26.
Davis |>
nest(data = -sex) |>
mutate(model = map(data, ~ lm(repwt ~ weight, data = .)),
tidied = map(model, tidy)) |>
unnest(tidied) |>
filter(term == "weight") |>
select(sex, term, estimate, std.error)
#> # A tibble: 2 × 4
#> sex term estimate std.error
#> <fct> <chr> <dbl> <dbl>
#> 1 M weight 0.990 0.0229
#> 2 F weight 0.262 0.0459
“What could be wrong here?”, the client asked. The consultant replied with the obvious question:
Did you plot your data?
The answer turned out to be one discrepant point, a female, whose measured weight was 166 kg (366 lbs!). This single point exerted so much influence that it pulled the fitted regression line down to a slope of only 0.26.
# shorthand to position legend inside the figure
legend_inside <- function(position) {
theme(legend.position = "inside",
legend.position.inside = position)
}
Davis |>
ggplot(aes(x = weight, y = repwt,
color = sex, shape = sex, linetype = sex)) +
geom_point(size = ifelse(Davis$weight==166, 6, 2)) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y~x, se = FALSE) +
labs(x = "Measured weight (kg)", y = "Reported weight (kg)") +
scale_linetype_manual(values = c(F = "longdash", M = "solid")) +
legend_inside(c(.8, .8))
In this example, it was arguable that \(x\) and \(y\) axes should be reversed, to determine how well measured weight can be predicted from reported weight. In ggplot
this can easily be done by reversing the x
and y
aesthetics.
Davis |>
ggplot(aes(y = weight, x = repwt, color = sex, shape=sex)) +
geom_point(size = ifelse(Davis$weight==166, 6, 2)) +
labs(y = "Measured weight (kg)", x = "Reported weight (kg)") +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y~x, se = FALSE) +
legend_inside(c(.8, .8))
In Figure 2.4, this discrepant observation again stands out like a sore thumb, but it makes very little difference in the fitted line for females. The reason is that this point is well within the range of the \(x\) variable (repwt
). To impact the slope of the regression line, an observation must be unusual in_both_ \(x\) and \(y\). We take up the topic of how to detect influential observations and what to do about them in Chapter 6.
The value of such plots is not only that they can reveal possible problems with an analysis, but also help identify their reasons and suggest corrective action. What went wrong here? Examination of the original data showed that this person switched the values, recording her reported weight in the box for measured weight and vice versa.
2.1.3 Shaken, not stirred: The 1970 Draft Lottery
Although we often hear that data speak for themselves, their voices can be soft and sly.—Frederick Mosteller (1983), Beginning Statistics with Data Analysis, p. 234.
The power of graphics is particularly evident when data contains a weak signal embedded in a field of noise. To the casual glance, there may seem to be nothing going on, but the signal can be made apparent in an incisive graph.
A dramatic example of this occurred in 1969 when the U.S. military conducted a lottery, the first since World War II, to determine which young men would be called up to serve in the Vietnam War for 1970. The U.S. Selective Service had devised a system to rank eligible men according to a random drawing of their birthdays. There were 366 blue plastic capsules containing birth dates placed in a transparent glass container and drawn by hand to assign ranked order-of-call numbers to all men within the 18-26 age range.
In an attempt to make the selection process also transparent, the proceeding was covered on radio, TV and film and the dates posted in order on a large display board. The first capsule—drawn by Congressman Alexander Pirnie (R-NY) of the House Armed Services Committee—contained the date September 14, so all men born on September 14 in any year between 1944 and 1950 were assigned lottery number 1, and would be drafted first. April 24 was drawn next, then December 30, February 14, and so on until June 8, selected last. At the time of the drawing, US officials stated that those with birthdays drawn in the first third would almost certainly be drafted, while those in the last third would probably avoid the draft (Fienberg, 1971).
I watched this unfold with considerable interest because I was eligible for the Draft that year. I was dismayed when my birthday, May 7, came up ranked 35. Ugh!
The data, from the official Selective Service listing are contained in the dataset vcdExtra::Draft1970
, ordered by Month
and birthdate (Day
), with Rank
as the order in which the birthdates were drawn.
library(ggplot2)
library(dplyr)
data(Draft1970, package = "vcdExtra")
dplyr::glimpse(Draft1970)
#> Rows: 366
#> Columns: 3
#> $ Day <int> 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 1…
#> $ Rank <int> 305, 159, 251, 215, 101, 224, 306, 199, 194, 325, 32…
#> $ Month <ord> Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Jan, Ja…
A basic scatterplot, slightly prettified, is shown in Figure 2.5. The points are colored by month, and month labels are shown at the bottom.
Show the code
# make markers for months at their mid points
months <- data.frame(
month =unique(Draft1970$Month),
mid = seq(15, 365-15, by = 30))
ggplot2:: theme_set(theme_bw(base_size = 16))
gg <- ggplot(Draft1970, aes(x = Day, y = Rank)) +
geom_point(size = 2.5, shape = 21,
alpha = 0.3,
color = "black",
aes(fill=Month)
) +
scale_fill_manual(values = rainbow(12)) +
geom_text(data=months, aes(x=mid, y=0, label=month), nudge_x = 5) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y ~ 1,
col = "black", fill="grey", linetype = "dashed", alpha=0.6) +
labs(x = "Day of the year",
y = "Lottery rank") +
theme(legend.position = "none")
gg
The ranks do seem to be essentially random. Is there any reason to suspect a flaw in the selection process, as I firmly hoped at the time?
If you stare at the graph in @fig-draft-gg1 long enough, you can make out a sparsity of points in the
upper right corner and also in the lower left corner compared to the opposite corners.
Visual smoothers
Fitting a linear regression line or a smoothed (loess) curve can bring out the signal lurking in the background of a field of nearly random points. Figure 2.6 shows a definite trend to lower ranks for birthdays toward the end of the year. Those born earlier in the year were more likely to be given lower ranks, calling them up sooner for the draft.
Show the code
ggplot(Draft1970, aes(x = Day, y = Rank)) +
geom_point(size = 2.5, shape = 21,
alpha = 0.3,
color = "black",
aes(fill=Month)) +
scale_fill_manual(values = rainbow(12)) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y~1,
se = FALSE,
col = "black", fill="grey", linetype = "dashed", alpha=0.6) +
geom_smooth(method = "loess", formula = y~x,
color = "blue", se = FALSE,
alpha=0.25) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y~x,
color = "darkgreen",
fill = "darkgreen",
alpha=0.25) +
geom_text(data=months, aes(x=mid, y=0, label=month), nudge_x = 5) +
labs(x = "Day of the year",
y = "Lottery rank") +
theme(legend.position = "none")
Is this a real effect? Even though the points seem to be random over the year, linear regression of Rank
on Day
shows a highly significant negative effect even though the correlation2 is small (\(r = -0.226\)). The slope, -0.226, means that for each additional day in the year the lottery rank decreases about 1/4 toward the front of the draft line; that’s nearly 7 ranks per month.
So, smoothing the data, using either the linear regression line or a nonparametric smoother is one important technique for seeing a weak signal in a noisy background.
Statistical summaries
Another way to enhance the signal-to-noise ratio of a graph is to plot summaries of the messy data points. For example, you might make boxplots of the ranks by month, or calculate and plot the mean or median rank by month and plot those together with some indication of variability within month.
Figure 2.7 plots the average Rank
for each month with error bars showing the mean \(\pm 1\) standard errors against the average Day
. The message of rank decreasing nearly linearly with month is now more dramatic. The correlation between the means is \(r = -0.867\).
Code
means <- Draft1970 |>
group_by(Month) |>
summarize(Day = mean(Day),
se = sd(Rank/ sqrt(n())),
Rank = mean(Rank))
ggplot(aes(x = Day, y = Rank), data=means) +
geom_point(size = 4) +
geom_smooth(method = "lm", formula = y~x,
color = "blue", fill = "blue", alpha = 0.1) +
geom_errorbar(aes(ymin = Rank-se, ymax = Rank+se), width = 8) +
geom_text(data=months, aes(x=mid, y=0, label=month), nudge_x = 5) +
labs(x = "Average day of the year",
y = "Average lottery rank")
The visual impression of a linearly decreasing trend in lottery rank is much stronger in Figure 2.7 than in Figure 2.6 for two reasons:
- Replacing the data points with their means strengthens the signal in relation to noise.
- The narrower vertical range (100–250) in the plot of means makes the slope of the line appear steeper. (However, the correlation of the means, \(r = -0.231\) is nearly the same as the correlation of the data points.)
What happened here?
Previous lotteries carried out by drawing capsules from a container had occasionally suffered the embarrassment that an empty capsule was selected because of vigorous mixing (Fienberg, 1971). So for the 1970 lottery, the birthdate capsules were put in cardboard boxes, one for each month and these were carefully emptied into the glass container in order of month: Jan., Feb., … Dec., gently shaken in atop the pile already there. All might have been well had the persons drawing the capsules put their hand in truly randomly, but generally they picked from toward the top of the container. Consequently, those born later in the year had a greater chance of being picked earlier.
There was considerable criticism of this procedure once the flaw had been revealed by analyses such as described here. In the following year, the Selective Service called upon the National Bureau of Standards to devise a better procedure. In 1971 they used two drums, one with the dates of the year and another with the rank numbers 1-366. As a date capsule was drawn randomly from the first drum, another from the numbers drum was picked simultaneously, giving a doubly-randomized sequence.
Of course, if they had R, the entire process could have been done using sample()
:
set.seed(42)
date = seq(as.Date("1971-01-01"), as.Date("1971-12-31"), by="+1 day")
rank = sample(seq_along(date))
draft1971 <- data.frame(date, rank)
head(draft1971, 4)
#> date rank
#> 1 1971-01-01 49
#> 2 1971-01-02 321
#> 3 1971-01-03 153
#> 4 1971-01-04 74
tail(draft1971, 4)
#> date rank
#> 362 1971-12-28 247
#> 363 1971-12-29 8
#> 364 1971-12-30 333
#> 365 1971-12-31 132
And, what would have happened to me and all others born on a May 7th, if they did it this way? My lottery rank would have 274!
me <- as.Date("1971-05-07")
draft1971[draft1971$date == me,]
#> date rank
#> 127 1971-05-07 274
2.2 Plots for data analysis
Visualization methods take an enormous variety of forms, but it is useful to distinguish several broad categories according to their use in data analysis:
data plots : primarily plot the raw data, often with annotations to aid interpretation (regression lines and smooths, data ellipses, marginal distributions)
reconnaissance plots : with more than a few variables, reconnaissance plots provide a high-level, bird’s-eye overview of the data, allowing you to see patterns that might not be visible in a set of separate plots. Some examples are scatterplot matrices, showing all bivariate plots of variables in a dataset; correlation diagrams, using visual glyphs to represent the correlations between all pairs of variables and “trellis” or faceted plots that show how a focal relation of one or more variables differs across values of other variables.
model plots : plot the results of a fitted model, such as a regression line or curve to show uncertainty, or a regression surface in 3D, or a plot of coefficients in model together with confidence intervals. Other model plots try to take into account that a fitted model may involve more variables than can be shown in a static 2D plot. Some examples of these are added variable plots, and marginal effect plots, both of which attempt to show the net relation of two focal variables, controlling or adjusting for other variables in a model.
diagnostic plots : indicating potential problems with the fitted model. These include residual plots, influence plots, plots for testing homogeneity of variance and so forth.
dimension reduction plots : plot representations of the data into a space of fewer dimensions than the number of variables in the dataset. Simple examples include principal components analysis (PCA) and the related biplots, and multidimensional scaling (MDS) methods.
We give some more details and a few examples in the sections that follow.
2.3 Data plots
Data plots portray the data in a space where the coordinate axes are the observed variables.
- 1D plots include line plots, histograms and density estimates.
- 2D plots are most often scatterplots, but contour plots or hex-binned plots are also useful when the sample size is large.
2.4 Model plots
Model plots show the fitted or predicted values from a statistical model and provide visual summaries…
2.5 Diagnostic plots
2.6 Principles of graphical display
[This could be a separate chapter]
- Criteria for assessing graphs: communication goals
- Effective data display:
- Make the data stand out
- Make graphical comparison easy
- Effect ordering: For variables and unordered factors, arrange them according to the effects to be seen
- Visual thinning: As the data becomes more complex, focus more on impactful summaries
Package summary
12 packages used here: broom, dplyr, forcats, ggplot2, knitr, lubridate, purrr, readr, stringr, tibble, tidyr, tidyverse